Charles Spence bibliography

 

Order your copy of Spence, C., & Driver, J. (Eds.) Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention.

Oxford: Oxford University Press at http://www.oup.co.uk/isbn/0-19-852486-2 now!

Amlôt, R., Walker, R., Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2003). Multimodal visual-somatosensory integration in saccade generation. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1-15.

Caclin, A., Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. (2002). Tactile ‘capture’ of audition. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 616-630.

Calvert, C., Spence, C., & Stein, B. E. (in press). An introduction to ‘The handbook of multisensory processes’. To appear in G. Calvert, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processing. MIT Press.

Calvert, G., Spence, C., & Stein, B. E. (Eds.) (2004). The handbook of multisensory processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chan, J. S., & Spence, C. (2003). Presenting multiple auditory signals using multiple sound cards in Visual Basic 6.0. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 125-128.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (in press). Crossmodal spatial attention: Evidence from human performance. In C. Spence & J. Driver (Eds.), Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (in press). Introductory bomments. In C. Spence & J. Driver (Eds.), Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2000). Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence. Current Biology, 10, R731-R735.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (1999). Crossmodal links in spatial attention. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and action (pp. 130-149). Oxford University Press.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (1998). Attention and the crossmodal construction of space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 254-262.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (1998). Crossmodal attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 245-253.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. (1998). Crossmodal links in spatial attention. Proceedings of the Royal Society Section B, 353, 1-13.

Driver, J., & Spence, C. J. (1994). Spatial synergies between auditory and visual attention. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and nonconcious information processing, (pp. 311-331). MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

Guest, S., & Spence, C. (2003). Tactile dominance in speeded discrimination of pilled fabric samples. Experimental Brain Research, 150, 201-207.

Guest. S., & Spence, C. (2003). What role does multisensory integration play in the visuotactile perception of texture? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 63-80.

Guest, S., Catmur, C., Lloyd, D., & Spence, C. (2002). Audiotactile interactions in roughness perception. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 161-171.

Hillstrom, A. P., Shapiro, K., & Spence, C. (2002). Attentional and perceptual limitations in processing sequentially presented vibrotactile targets. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 1068-1082.

Kennett, S., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2002). Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 1083-1094.

Kennett, S., Eimer, M., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2001). Tactile-visual links in exogenous spatial attention under different postures: Convergent evidence from psychophysics and ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 462-478.

Lloyd, D. M., Merat, N., McGlone, F., & Spence, C. (2003). Crossmodal links between audition and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 901-924.

Lloyd, D. M., Shore, D. I., Spence, C., & Calvert, G. A. (2003). Multisensory representation of limb position in human premotor cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 17- 18.

Macaluso, E., George, N., Driver, J., Dolan, R., & Spence, C. (in press). Spatiotemporal contributions to audiovisual speech perception: A PET study. Neuroimage.

Maravita, A., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2003). Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach. Current Biology, 13, R531- R539.

Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83, B25-B34.

Maravita, A., Spence, C., Sergent, C., & Driver, J. (2002). Seeing your own touched hands in a mirror modulates cross-modal interactions. Psychological Science, 13, 350-356.

Maravita, A., Spence, C., Clarke, K., Husain, M., & Driver, J. (2000). Vision and touch through the looking glass in a case of crossmodal extinction. NeuroReport, 11, 3521-3526.

Shore, D. I., Spry, E., & Spence, C. (2002). Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 153-163.

Shore, D. I., Spence, C., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. Psychological Science, 12, 205-212.

Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. (in press). Cross-modal dynamic capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

Soto-Faraco, S., Ronald, A., & Spence, C. (in press). Tactile selective attention and body posture: Assessing the contribution of vision and proprioception. Perception & Psychophysics.

Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C., Lloyd, D., & Kingstone, A. (in press). Moving multisensory research along: Motion perception across sensory modalities. Current Directions in Psychological Science.

Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. (2003). Multisensory contributions to the perception of motion. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1847-1862.

Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2002). Modality-specific auditory and visual temporal processing deficits. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (A), 55, 23-40.

Soto-Faraco, S., Lyons, J., Gazzaniga, M., Spence, C., & Kingstone, A. (2002). The ventriloquist in motion: Illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 139-146.

Soto-Faraco, S. Spence, C., Fairbank, K., Kingstone, A., Hillstrom, A. P., & Shapiro, K. (2002). A crossmodal attentional blink between vision and touch. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 731-738.

Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2001). Spatial modulation of repetition blindness and repetition deafness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1181-1202.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (Eds.) (2004). Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Spence, C., & McDonald, J. (in press). The crossmodal consequences of the exogenous spatial orienting of attention To appear in G. Calvert, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Spence, C., McDonald, J., & Driver, J. (in press). Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration. In C. Spence & J. Driver (Eds.), Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Spence, C., Pavani, F., Maravita, A., & Holmes, N. (in press). Multisensory contributions to the 3-D representation of visuotactile peripersonal space in humans: Evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Journal of Physiology (Paris).

Spence, C. (2003). Crossmodal attention and multisensory integration: Implications for multimodal interface design. ICMI’03. Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (November 5- 7, Vancouver, British Columbia, CA; p. 3). New York: The Association for Computing Machinery Press.

Spence, C., & Read, L. (2003). Speech shadowing while driving: On the difficulty of splitting attention between eye and ear. Psychological Science, 14, 251-256.

Spence, C., & Squire, S. B. (2003). Multisensory integration: Maintaining the perception of synchrony. Current Biology, 13, R519-R521.

Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Crossmodal temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 318-328.

Spence, C. (2002). Multimodal attention and tactile information-processing. Behavioural Brain Research, 135, 57-64.

Spence, C. (2002). Multisensory integration, attention and perception. In D. Roberts (Ed.), Signals and perception: The fundamentals of human sensation (pp. 345-354). Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

Spence, C. (2002). Review of ‘Mechanisms of Visual Attention’, by Schneider & Maasen. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (B), 55, 89-90.

Spence, C. (2002). The ICI report on the secret of the senses. London: The Communication Group.

Spence, C., Bentley, D. E., Phillips, N., McGlone, F. P., & Jones, A. K. P. (2002). Selective attention to pain: A psychophysical investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 395-402.

Spence, C. (2001). Crossmodal attentional capture: A controversy resolved? In C. Folk & B. Gibson (Eds.), Attention, distraction and action: Multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 231-262). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science BV.

Spence, C., & McGlone, F. P. (2001). Reflexive spatial orienting of tactile attention. Experimental Brain Research, 141, 324-330.

Spence, C., Shore, D. I., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Multimodal prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 799-832.

Spence, C., Kingstone, A., Shore, D. I., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2001). Representation of visuotactile space in the split brain. Psychological Science, 12, 90-93.

Spence, C., Kettenmann, B., Kobal, G., & McGlone, F. P. (2001). Attention to olfaction: A psychophysical investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 138, 432- 437.

Spence, C., Kettenmann, B., Kobal, G., & McGlone, F. P. (2001). Shared attentional resources for processing vision and chemosensation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 775-783.

Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2001). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 330-336.

Spence, C., Shore, D. I., Gazzaniga, M. S., Soto, S., & Kingstone, A. (2001). Failure to remap visuotactile space across the midline in the split-brain. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 54-61.

Spence, C., Guest, S, Chan, C., Lloyd, D., McGlone, F., Phillips, N., & Jones, L. A. (2001). Fooling the senses. Royal Society New Frontiers in Science Exhibition Brochure.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Attracting attention to the illusory location of a sound: Reflexive crossmodal orienting and ventriloquism. NeuroReport, 11, 2057- 2061.

Spence, C., Ranson, J., & Driver, J. (2000). Crossmodal selective attention: Ignoring auditory stimuli presented at the focus of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 410-424.

Spence, C., Pavani, F., & Driver, J. (2000). Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1298-1319.

Spence, C., Kettenmann, B., Kobal, G., & McGlone, F. P. (2000). Selective attention to the chemosensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1265- 1271.

Spence, C., Lloyd, D., McGlone, F., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2000). Inhibition of return is supramodal: A demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 42-48.

Spence, C. (1999). The psychology of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 41.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1999). Multiple resources and multimodal interface design. In D. Harris (Ed.), Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, Vol. 3: Transportation Systems, Medical Ergonomics and Training (pp. 305-312). Ashgate Publishing: Hampshire.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1999). A new approach to the design of multimodal warning signals. In D. Harris (Ed.), Engineering psychology and cognitive ergonomics, Vol. 4: Job design, product design and human- computer interaction (pp. 455-461). Ashgate Publishing: Hampshire.

Spence, C. (1998). What’s the difference between attention and black holes? Applied Cognitive Psychology.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1998). Auditory and audiovisual inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 125-139.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1998). Crossmodal links between auditory and visual attention. In P. K. Kuhl, & L. A. Crum (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress on Acoustics and the 135th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America (Vol. 3, pp.1569- 1570). ASA: NY.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1998). Inhibition of return following an auditory cue: The role of central reorienting events. Experimental Brain Research, 118, 352-360.

Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E. R., Gillespie, N., & Driver, J. (1998). Cross-modal links in exogenous covert spatial orienting between touch, audition, and vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 544-557.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1-22.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). Audiovisual links in attention: Implications for interface design. In D. Harris (Ed.), Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, Vol.2., Job Design and Product Design (pp. 185-192). Ashgate Publishing: Hampshire.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). Cross-modal links in attention between audition, vision, and touch: Implications for interface design. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 1, 351-373.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). On measuring selective attention to a specific sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 389-403.

Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1996). Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1005-1030.

Spence, C. J., & Driver, J. (1994). Covert spatial orienting in audition: Exogenous and endogenous mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 555-574.

Spence, C., Bösser, T., & Buskens, C. (1991). Crew work station and module control staion evaluation. European Space Agency Contractors Report, WGO/876/FS.

Spence, C., Spiegel, H., Goulet, D., Gunsthovel, D., & Bösser, T. (1992). Analytical measures for usability evaluation. Esprit Music Project Technical Report, 5429.

Pavani, F., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Visual capture of touch; Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychological Science, 11, 353-359.

Poliakoff, E., Spence, C., O’Boyle D. J., McGlone F. P., & Cody, F. W. J. (2002). Tactile inhibition of return: Non- ocular response inhibition and mode of response. Experimental Brain Research, 146, 54-59.

Poliakoff, E., O’Boyle, D. J., Moore, P.,Cody, F. W. J., McGlone, F., & Spence, C. (2003). Orienting of attention and Parkinson’s disease: Tactile inhibition of return and response inhibition. Brain, 126, 2081-2092.

Röder, B., Spence, C., & Rösler, F. (2002). Assessing the effect of posture changes on tactile inhibition of return. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 453-462.

Röder, B., Spence, C., & Rösler, F. (2000). Inhibition of return and oculomotor control in the blind. NeuroReport, 11, 3043-3045.

Walton, M., & Spence, C. (in press). Tactile ventriloquism, visual dominance, and stimulus salience. Experimental Brain Research.

Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (in press). Multisensory contribution to food perception: The role of auditory cues in modulating crispness and staleness in crisps. Journal of Sensory Science.

Zampini, M., Guest, S., & Spence, C. (2003). The role of auditory cues in modulating the perception of electric toothbrushes. Journal of Dental Research, 82, 929- 932.

Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003). Audiovisual temporal order judgments. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 198-210.

Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: The role of hemispheric redundancy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 165-180.